top of page
Search

Learn Phonetics and Phonology with The Sounds Of Language Zsiga Pdf 12

  • pocorrabapegthea
  • Aug 19, 2023
  • 6 min read


Sound symbolism refers to the non-arbitrary mappings that exist between phonetic properties of speech sounds and their meaning. Despite there being an extensive literature on the topic, the acoustic features and psychological mechanisms that give rise to sound symbolism are not, as yet, altogether clear. The present study was designed to investigate whether different sets of acoustic cues predict size and shape symbolism, respectively. In two experiments, participants judged whether a given consonant-vowel speech sound was large or small, round or angular, using a size or shape scale. Visual size judgments were predicted by vowel formant F1 in combination with F2, and by vowel duration. Visual shape judgments were, however, predicted by formants F2 and F3. Size and shape symbolism were thus not induced by a common mechanism, but rather were distinctly affected by acoustic properties of speech sounds. These findings portray sound symbolism as a process that is not based merely on broad categorical contrasts, such as round/unround and front/back vowels. Rather, individuals seem to base their sound-symbolic judgments on specific sets of acoustic cues, extracted from speech sounds, which vary across judgment dimensions.




The Sounds Of Language Zsiga Pdf 12




Which acoustic vowel features might drive sound-symbolic judgments? Sound-size symbolism should be associated with acoustic features that express size and/or intensity. For instance, among individuals of (mammal) species, greater physical size is commonly associated with a lower fundamental frequency f0 in vocalizations28, 29. f0 also varies across the different speech sounds produced by an individual. To the extent that intra-individual changes in f0 are interpreted similarly to inter-individual changes in terms of their implied size, we therefore predict speech sounds with lower f0 would lead to increased size judgments. The latter should also be affected by formants that indicate greater opening of the oral cavity in an iconic way, as larger opening represents larger size. Therefore, the first formant F1, which increases with lower tongue position and greater jaw opening, should be positively related to visual size judgments. Finally, intensity-related features such as loudness and duration might also affect size judgments. As higher-intensity sounds tend to correspond to larger objects than sounds of lower intensity28, 30,31,32, the intrinsic loudness of vowels should also be associated with the size of an object. Thus, vowels having a higher intensity should correspond to larger objects than vowels with a lower intensity. Similarly, vowels with a longer duration should be associated with larger objects than vowels that are associated with a shorter duration.


Which acoustic vowel properties will have the greatest influence on sound-shape-symbolic judgments, such as judgments of visual roundness versus angularity? Spectral features reflecting lip rounding may influence sound-shape symbolism due to the perceptual analogy between lip rounding and visual roundness. In acoustic terms, lip rounding lengthens the entire vocal tract and therefore lowers all formants, especially F2 and F3. Backing and rounding have reinforcing acoustic effects, as both lower F233. We therefore predict that sounds with a lower F2 and F3 will be associated with more rounded shapes, while sounds with a higher F2 and F3 will be associated with more angular shapes instead.


In sum, the present study suggests that different acoustic drivers underlie size and shape symbolism. Specifically, F1 in combination with F2, and duration predicted size symbolism, while shape symbolism was associated with F2 and F3. These findings portray sound symbolism as a process that is not merely based on categorical contrasts, such as the differentiation between round and unround or front and back vowels. Instead, individuals base their sound-symbolic judgments on specific sets of acoustic cues extracted from the sounds, which vary across judgment dimensions.


Further research could extend the comparative methodology of the present study to additional sound-meaning correspondences, such as sound and weight, taste, or emotion, and extend the comparisons to children of different age groups. Tracking the development of different types of sound symbolism and cross-modal correspondences is crucial for our understanding of the nature versus nurture debate on sound symbolism. Studies using more implicit methods, such as artificial learning tasks51, 52, may reveal more about the role of sound-symbolic effects in natural language learning and processing. There are also scant neuroimaging studies on the topic sound symbolism, with those that have been published mainly focusing on the existence of sound-symbolic effects in adults8 and children13. An EEG study with children confirmed increased processing demands in sound-meaning mismatch conditions13, and an fMRI study with adults located the left superior parietal cortex as the potential site for sound-symbolic mapping8. Extending this existing literature with the current results in mind, we suggest further neuroimaging studies that specifically compare the brain activation patterns for different types of sound-symbolic judgments to help uncover the neural basis underlying different types of sound symbolism.


Several acoustic properties of the auditory stimuli were measured using Praat (www.praat.org): We first segmented the consonant and the vowel of the 100 CV sounds, then we measured f0 and formants F1-F3 in the middle of the vowel, peak intensity, and duration of the vowel. Our acoustic analyses were focused exclusively on the acoustic features of vowels, because consonants do not have common acoustic parameters; rather, the most important acoustic features of consonants vary across different consonant classes.


The study of language and the history of language is an interesting subject. Over time, any language can go through many different phonotactic variations and changes. Today, we will be looking at understanding phonotactics and its modern-day constraints and some examples of phonotactics in phonology.


In particular, English has conditions placed on it that native speakers learn inherently without necessarily realizing it. This is why, when non-native speakers are learning the language, it may be important to include this in your structured literacy program.


In The Routledge Dictionary of English Language Studies, Michael Pearce pointed out that some languages, such as English, allow for clusters of consonants. Meanwhile, other languages, such as Maori, do not.


In English, these consonant clusters are subject to phonotactic constraints that control the length, what sequences of words are allowed, and where syllables can occur in a sentence. This is often why learning English as a second language can be frustrating when your first language does not allow consonant clusters.


Eva Fernández and Helen Smith Cairns pointed out in Fundamentals of Psycholinguistics that some phonotactic constraints are universal, particularly in syllable structure. All languages contain syllables made up of a consonant and then a vowel, but different languages are more specific in their constraints.


We have many different rules and constraints that determine which words go where in a sentence. Some are now arbitrary thanks to a lack of updating the rules as our languages have evolved. At the same time, some are important for keeping some semblance of rigidity when mastering a language.


Examining classic and current linguistic theories of how physical and cognitive factors interact in the mind of the speaker, and in the language system as a whole, Elizabeth Zsiga provides a rigorous guide to the key debates for the advanced student.


In our current study, we focus on the learnability and flexibility of coarticulatory timing patterns (patterns of temporal overlap) between successive consonants. While consonant timing is usually not considered to be a locus of category contrast, it is well known that languages generally have different constraints on how successive articulatory gestures of consonants or consonants and vowels overlap in time (Bombien & Hoole, 2013; Hermes Mücke, & Auris, 2017). We refer to this overlap as coarticulatory timing (for an overview of coarticulatory patterns arising from the temporal overlap see e.g., Farnetani & Recasens, 2010). Coarticulatory timing is part of native speaker knowledge and can be seen as part of a language-specific grammar of coarticulation. While quite a number of publications are concerned with phonotactic learning in perception and production (among many others, Davidson, 2006; Goldrick & Larson, 2008; Redford, 2008; Seidl, Onishi, & Cristia, 2013), less is known about how flexibly adult speakers can adapt the phonetic detail of consonant sequences to unfamiliar coarticulatory timing patterns, and it is this question that our study addresses.


The particular coarticulatory timing differences that are at the focus of this paper relate to the amount of temporal overlap between successive consonants forming an onset cluster, a known locus of cross-linguistic differences: For instance, the two consonants of an onset cluster overlap more in German compared to French (Bombien & Hoole, 2013). In languages such as Spanish (Hall, 2006), Norwegian (Endresen, 1991), Russian (Zsiga, 2003), or Georgian (Chitoran, 1998; Chitoran, Goldstein, & Byrd, 2002), consonant sequences may be produced with a so-called open transition. Such an open transition arises if the constriction for the second consonant is still being formed while the constriction for the first consonant has already been released (Catford, 1985). An open transition can be defined as a period of sound radiation from an open vocal tract between two constrictions (see e.g., Figure 10b). This may under certain circumstances, depending on glottal aperture and aerodynamic conditions, lead to the emergence of an excrescent vocoid. It is generally understood that the acoustics of the transition are purely contextually conditioned but separating transitional vocoids from epenthetic vowels with a vocalic constriction target is notoriously difficult (e.g., Davidson, 2005; Ridouane, 2008). The work we report here does not crucially depend on such a distinction, and we return to this point in the Discussion. 2ff7e9595c


 
 
 

Recent Posts

See All
Youtube apk 5.1 download

Download do APK 5.1 do YouTube: como obter a versão mais recente do YouTube no seu dispositivo Android O YouTube é uma das plataformas de...

 
 
 

Comments


500 Terry Francois Street

San Francisco, CA 94158

Tel: 123-456-7890

Mon - Fri: 7am - 10pm

Saturday: 8am - 11pm

Sunday: 8am - 10pm

Let's be friends and have eggs benedict

Thanks for submitting!

© 2023 by White and Yellow. Proudly created with Wix.com

bottom of page